Powered by OpenAIRE graph
Found an issue? Give us feedback
addClaim

This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.

You have already added 0 works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.

An Experiment in Open Economics

Authors: Ann Coates; Dennis M. O'Toole;

An Experiment in Open Economics

Abstract

4-option questions, but no such overall tendency for 5-option questions.3 Forty-five percent of the sources were biased in their use of this option, but they were skewed in both directions, thus tending to offset one another in the summary study-one-third strongly favored that choice and two-thirds pointedly avoided it. There was a strong bias towards the "all of the above" choice in both 4-option and 5-option questions: 69 percent correct for the former and 54 percent correct for the latter, where the expected values are 25 and 20 percent, respectively (see Table 1). Eighty percent, of the individual sources favored this as the correct option. For true-false questions, since a "true" statement may appear obviously true to the author, he may have a bias towards writing "false" statements. Moreover, due to the structure of the question, the statement must be completely true to qualify as "true," whereas if it is partly false, the correct choice is "false." Thus, one might expect a bias towards "false" answers. The overall data on true-false frequencies rejected our initial hypothesis. Instead, there was a mild bias towards the "true" option.4 The aggregate data, however, concealed considerable bias among individual authors. Twelve of the sources (41 percent) indicated significant bias: 7 favored the "true" and 5 the "false" option. Some authors were very sharply skewed from the norm on true-false questions. Since an examination is a sample of a student's knowledge, the validity of that sample is diminished if it contains the type of bias described above. Users may correct this deficiency through careful selection, rearrangement, or rewriting, while a careless user may increase the bias.

  • BIP!
    Impact byBIP!
    citations
    This is an alternative to the "Influence" indicator, which also reflects the overall/total impact of an article in the research community at large, based on the underlying citation network (diachronically).
    0
    popularity
    This indicator reflects the "current" impact/attention (the "hype") of an article in the research community at large, based on the underlying citation network.
    Average
    influence
    This indicator reflects the overall/total impact of an article in the research community at large, based on the underlying citation network (diachronically).
    Average
    impulse
    This indicator reflects the initial momentum of an article directly after its publication, based on the underlying citation network.
    Average
Powered by OpenAIRE graph
Found an issue? Give us feedback
citations
This is an alternative to the "Influence" indicator, which also reflects the overall/total impact of an article in the research community at large, based on the underlying citation network (diachronically).
BIP!Citations provided by BIP!
popularity
This indicator reflects the "current" impact/attention (the "hype") of an article in the research community at large, based on the underlying citation network.
BIP!Popularity provided by BIP!
influence
This indicator reflects the overall/total impact of an article in the research community at large, based on the underlying citation network (diachronically).
BIP!Influence provided by BIP!
impulse
This indicator reflects the initial momentum of an article directly after its publication, based on the underlying citation network.
BIP!Impulse provided by BIP!
0
Average
Average
Average
Upload OA version
Are you the author? Do you have the OA version of this publication?