The science of psychoanalysis

Article English OPEN
Lacewing, Michael (2018)

For psychoanalysis to qualify as scientific psychology, it needs to generate data that can evidentially support theoretical claims. Its methods, therefore, must at least be capable of correcting for biases produced in the data during the process of generating it; and we must be able to use the data in sound forms of inference and reasoning. Critics of psychoanalysis have claimed that it fails on both counts, and thus whatever warrant its claims have derive from other sources. In this article, I discuss three key objections, and then consider their implications together with recent developments in the generation and testing of psychoanalytic theory. The first and most famous is that of ‘suggestion’; if it sticks, clinical data may be biased in a way that renders all inferences from them unreliable. The second, sometimes confused with the first, questions whether the data are or can be used to provide genuine tests of theoretical hypotheses. The third will require us to consider the question of how psychology can reliably infer motives from behavior. I argue that the clinical method of psychoanalysis is defensible against these objections in relation to the psychodynamic model of mind, but not wider metapsychological and etiological claims. Nevertheless, the claim of psychoanalysis to be a science would be strengthened if awareness of the methodological pitfalls and means to avoid them, and alternative theories and their evidence bases, were more widespread. This may require changes in the education of psychoanalysts.
  • References (90)
    90 references, page 1 of 9

    Andersen, S. M., & Chen, S. (2002). The relational self: An interpersonal social-cognitive theory. Psychological Review, 109(4), 619-645.

    Andersen, S. M., & Glassman, N. S. (1996). Responding to significant others when they are not there: Effects on interpersonal inference, motivation, and affect. In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition (Vol. 3, pp. 262-321). New York: Guilford Press.

    Andersen, S. M., & Thorpe, J. S. (2009). An IF-THEN theory of personality: Significant others and the relational self. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(2), 163-170.

    Bargh, J. (2005). Bypassing the will: toward demystifying the nonconscious control of social behaviour. In In: Hassin, R.R., Uleman J.S., and Bargh J.A. (Eds). The New Unconscious, 37-58. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Brearley, M. 2008. What do psychoanalysts do? In The academic face of psychoanalysis, ed. L. Braddock and M. Lacewing, 20-32. London: Routledge.

    Conte, H. and Plutchik, R. (eds) (1995). Ego defenses: Theory and measurement. New York: John Wiley.

    Cramer, P. (2006). Protecting the self: Defense mechanisms in action. New York: Guilford Press.

    Dahl, H. (1972). A quantitative study of a psychoanalysis. Psychoanalysis and Contemporary Science, 1(1), 237-257.

    Dahl, H., Kächele, H., and Thomä, H. (1988). Psychoanalytic Process Research Strategies. Berlin: Springer.

    Eagle, M. (1997). Attachment and psychoanalysis. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 70(3), 217- 229.

  • Metrics
    0
    views in OpenAIRE
    0
    views in local repository
    773
    downloads in local repository

    The information is available from the following content providers:

    From Number Of Views Number Of Downloads
    Heythrop College Publications - IRUS-UK 0 773
Share - Bookmark