When Lying Feels the Right Thing to Do

Article English OPEN
Van Der Zee, Sophie ; Anderson, Ross ; Poppe, Ronald (2016)
  • Publisher: Frontiers Media S.A.
  • Journal: Frontiers in Psychology (issn: 1664-1078, vol: 7)
  • Related identifiers: doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00734, pmc: PMC4890549
  • Subject: MTurk | Psychology | deception | dishonesty | Z415 | Original Research | Z261 | insurance fraud | Z471 | rejection

Fraud is a pervasive and challenging problem that costs society large amounts of money. By no means all fraud is committed by ‘professional criminals’: much is done by ordinary people who indulge in small-scale opportunistic deception. In this paper, we set out to investigate when people behave dishonestly, for example by committing fraud, in an online context. We conducted three studies to investigate how the rejection of one’s efforts, operationalized in different ways, affected the amount of cheating and information falsification. Study 1 demonstrated that people behave more dishonestly when rejected. Studies 2 and 3 were conducted in order to disentangle the confounding factors of the nature of the rejection and the financial rewards that are usually associated with dishonest behavior. It was demonstrated that rejection in general, rather than the nature of a rejection, caused people to behave more dishonestly. When a rejection was based on subjective grounds, dishonest behavior increased with approximately 10%, but this difference was not statistically significant. We subsequently measured whether dishonesty was driven by the financial loss associated with rejection, or emotional factors such as a desire for revenge. We found that rejected participants were just as dishonest when their cheating did not led to financial gain. However, they felt stronger emotions when there was no money involved. This seems to suggest that upon rejection, emotional involvement, especially a reduction in happiness, drives dishonest behavior more strongly than a rational cost-benefit analysis. These results indicate that rejection causes people to behave more dishonestly, specifically in online settings. Firms wishing to deter customers and employees from committing fraud may therefore benefit from transparency and clear policy guidelines, discouraging people to submit claims that are likely to be rejected.
  • References (52)
    52 references, page 1 of 6

    Alanezi, F., and Brooks, L. (2014). “Combatting online fraud in Saudi Arabia using general deterrence theory,” in Proceeding of the 20th Americas Conference on Information Systems, Savannah, GA.

    Anderson, R., Barton, C., Böhme, R., Clayton, R., van Eeten, M. J. G., and Levi, M. (2012). “The economics of information security and privacy,” in Measuring the Cost of Cybercrime, eds R. Böhme (Berlin: Springer)

    Ariely, D. (2012). The Honest Truth about Dishonesty: How we lie to Everyone - Especially Ourselves. New York, NY: Harper Collins.

    Aronson, E. (1969). “The theory of cognitive dissonance: a current perspective,” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, ed. L. Berkowitz (New York, NY: Academic Press), 1-34.

    Barkan, R., Ayal, S., and Ariely, D. (2015). Ethical dissonance, justifications, and moral behavior. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 6, 157-161. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.08.001

    Barter, C., Renold, E., Berridge, D., and Cawson, P. (2004). Peer Violence in Children's Residential Care. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Baumeister, R. F., and Tice, D. M. (1990). Point-counterpoints: anxiety and social exclusion. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 9, 165-195. doi: 10.1521/jscp.1990.9. 2.165

    Berinsky, A. J., Huber, G. A., and Lenz, G. S. (2012). Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: Amazon.com's Mechanical Turk. Polit. Anal. 20, 351-368. doi: 10.1093/pan/mpr057

    Bies, R. J., and Shapiro, D. L. (1987). Interactional fairness judgments: the influence of causal accounts. Soc. Justice Res. 1, 199-218. doi: 10.1007/BF01048016

    Blasi, A. (1980). Bridging moral cognition and moral action: a critical review of the literature. Psychol. Bull. 88, 1-45. doi: 10.1007/s10979-009-9185-9

  • Similar Research Results (4)
  • Metrics
    No metrics available
Share - Bookmark