Non-word repetition impairment in autism and specific language impairment: Evidence for distinct underlying neuro-cognitive causes

Article English OPEN
Williams, David M. ; Payne, Heather ; Marshall, Chloe (2013)
  • Publisher: Kluwer Academic Publishers-Plenum Publishers
  • Related identifiers: doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1579-8
  • Subject: BF
    mesheuropmc: behavioral disciplines and activities | mental disorders

Language-impaired individuals with autism perform poorly on tests such as non-word repetition that are sensitive clinical markers of specific language impairment (SLI). This has fuelled the theory that language impairment in autism represents co-morbid SLI. However, the underlying cause of these deficits may be different in each disorder. In a novel task, we manipulated non-word stimuli in three ways known to influence the repetition accuracy of children with SLI. Participants with SLI were affected differently by these manipulations to children with autism. Children with autism performed similarly to language-matched typical children in terms of levels and patterns of performance, and types of error made, suggesting that the underlying cognitive cause of non-word repetition deficits is different in each disorder.
  • References (46)
    46 references, page 1 of 5

    Archibald, L.M., & Gathercole, S. E. (2007). Nonword repetition in specific language impairment: More than a phonological short-term memory deficit. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14, 919-924.

    Archibald, L.M., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition: A comparison of tests. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 970-983.

    Arking, D.E., Cutler, D.J., Brune, C. W., Teslovich, T. M., West, K., Ikeda, M., et al. (2008). A common genetic variant in the neurexin superfamily member CNTNAP2 increases familial risk of autism. American Journal of Human Genetics, 82, 160-164.

    Baird, G., Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., Meldrum, D., et al. (2006). Prevalence of disorders of the autism spectrum in a population cohort of children in South Thames: the Special Needs and Autism Project (SNAP). Lancet, 368(9531), 210-215.

    Ballarati, L., Recalcati, M. P., Bedeschi, M. F., Lalatta, F., Valtorta, C., Bellini, M., Finelli, P., Larizza, L., Giardino, D. (2009). Cytogenetic, FISH and array-CGH characterization of a complex chromosomal rearrangement carried by a mentally and language impaired patient. European Journal of Medical Genetics, 52, 218 -223.

    Barry, J. G., Yasin, I., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2007). Heritable risk factors associated with language impairments. Genes Brain and Behavior, 6, 66-76.

    Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A. (1975). Comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorder 1: Children. British Journal of Psychiatry, 126, 127-145.

    Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A. (1977). Comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorder 3: Discriminant function analysis. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 7(4), 383-396.

    Bishop, D.V.M., Maybery, M., Wong, D., Maley, A., Hill, W., & Hallmayer, J. (2004). Are phonological processing deficits part of the broad autism phenotype? American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 128B, 54-60.

    Bishop, D.V.M., North, T., & Donlan, C. (1996). Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: Evidence from a twin study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 37, 391-403.

  • Metrics
    0
    views in OpenAIRE
    0
    views in local repository
    85
    downloads in local repository

    The information is available from the following content providers:

    From Number Of Views Number Of Downloads
    Kent Academic Repository - IRUS-UK 0 85
Share - Bookmark