The goal circuit model: a hierarchical multi-route model of the acquisition and control of routine sequential action in humans

Article English OPEN
Cooper, Richard P. ; Ruh, N. ; Mareschal, Denis (2014)

Human control of action in routine situations involves a flexible interplay between (a) task dependent serial ordering constraints, (b) top-down, or intentional, control processes and (c) bottom-up, or environmentally-triggered, affordances. Additionally, the interaction between these influences is modulated by learning mechanisms that, over time, appear to reduce the need for top-down control processes while still allowing those processes to intervene at any point if necessary or if desired. We present a model of the acquisition and control of goal-directed action that goes beyond existing models by operationalizing an interface between two putative systems – a routine and a non-routine system – thereby demonstrating how explicitly represented goals can interact with the emergent task representations that develop through learning in the routine system. The gradual emergence of task representations offers an explanation for the transfer of control with experience from the non-routine goal-based system to the routine system. At the same time it allows action selection to be sensitive both to environmental triggers and to biasing from multiple levels within the goal system.
  • References (67)
    67 references, page 1 of 7

    Altmann, E. M., & Trafton, G. J. (2002). Memory for goals: an activation-based model. Cognition, 26, 39-83.

    Beldarrain, M. G., Grafman, J., Pascual-Leone, A., & Garcia-Monco, J. C. (1999). Procedural learning is impaired in patients with prefrontal lesions. Neurology, 52(9), 1853-1868.

    Botvinick, M. M., & Bylsma, L. M. (2005). Distraction and action slips in an everyday task: Evidence for a dynamic representation of task context. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(6), 1011-1017.

    Botvinick, M. M., & Plaut, D. C. (2004). Doing without schema hierarchies: A recurrent connectionist approach to normal and impaired routine sequential action. Psychological Review, 111(2), 395-429.

    Botvinick, M. M. & Plaut, D. C. (2006). Such stuff as habits are made on: A reply to Cooper and Shallice (2006). Psychological Review, 113(4), 917-927.

    Botvinick, M. M., Niv, Y., & Barto, A. C. (2009). Hierarchically organized behavior and its neural foundations: A reinforcement learning perspective. Cognition, 113, 262-280.

    Braver, T. S. & Cohen, J. D. (2000). On the control of control: the role of dopamine in regulating prefrontal function and working memory. In S. Monsell and J. S. Driver (eds.), Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII (pp. 713-737). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Brooks, R. A. (1991). How to build complete creatures rather than isolated cognitive simulators. In K. VanLehn (Ed.), Architectures for Intelligence (pp. 225-239). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Cassimatis, N. L., Bello, P., & Langley, P. (2008). Ability, breadth, and parsimony in computational models of higher-order cognition. Cognitive Science, 32, 1304-1322.

    Cooper, R. P., Fox, J., Farringdon, J., & Shallice, T. (1996). A systematic methodology for cognitive modelling. Artificial Intelligence, 85, 3-44.

  • Metrics
    0
    views in OpenAIRE
    0
    views in local repository
    29
    downloads in local repository

    The information is available from the following content providers:

    From Number Of Views Number Of Downloads
    Birkbeck Institutional Research Online - IRUS-UK 0 29
Share - Bookmark