Self-deception can evolve under appropriate costs

Article English OPEN
Ramirez, J.C. ; Marshall, J.A.R. (2015)
  • Publisher: Current Zoology
  • References (30)
    30 references, page 1 of 3

    Alicke MD, Govorun O, 2005. The better-than-average effect. In: Alicke MD, Dunning DA, Krueger J ed. The Self in Social Judgment: Studies in Self and Identity. New York: Psychology Press, 85-106.

    Backwell PR, Christy JH, Telford SR, Jennions MD, Passmore NI, 2000. Dishonest signaling in a fiddler crab. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Biological Sciences 267: 719-24.

    Baker JE, 1987. Reducing bias and inefficiency in the selection algorithm. In: Grefenstette JJ ed. Proceedings of the Second International Conference on Genetic Algorithms. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates, 14-21.

    Bandura A, Brooks ML, SwannWB, Jr., Buss DM, Dunning D et al., 2011. Open Peer Commentary on The evolution and psychology of self-deception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34: 16-41.

    Brooks ML, Swann WB, 2011. Is social interaction based on guile or honesty? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34: 17-18.

    Chambers JR, Windschitl PD, 2004. Biases in social comparative judgments: The role of nonmotivated factors in above-average and comparative-optimism effects. Psychological Bulletin 130: 813-838.

    Dougherty MRP, Gettys CF, Ogden EE, 1999. MINERVA-DM: A memory processes model for judgments of likelihood. Psychological Review 106: 180-209.

    Frey U, Voland E, 2011. The evolutionary route to self-deception: Why offensive versus defensive strategy might be a false alternative. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34: 21-22.

    Funder DC, 2011. Directions and beliefs of self-presentational bias. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34: 23.

    Johnson DDP, Fowler JH, 2011. The evolution of overconfidence. Nature 477: 317-20.

  • Metrics
    No metrics available
Share - Bookmark